Lie Detector Tests | Encyclopedia.com (2024)

JULI BERWALD

A polygraph test is administered to determine whether or not statements made by the subject taking the test are deceptive. During the test, the subject is monitored by a polygraph machine and interrogated by an administrator trained in forensic psychophysiology. The machine measures changes in the subject's blood pressure, heart rate, respiration rate and sweat production. The theory underlying the polygraph test is that a person who is lying exhibits involuntary physiological responses that can be detected by the polygraph instrument. These changes include rapid breathing and heartbeat and increased blood pressure and perspiration.

The Polygraph Instrument

The polygraph instrument usually measures four to six physiological reactions recorded by three different medical instruments that are combined in one machine. Older polygraph machines were equipped with long strips of paper that moved slowly beneath pens that recorded the various physiological responses. Newer equipment uses transducers to convert the information to digital signals that can be stored on computers and analyzed using sophisticated mathematical algorithms.

The three components of the polygraph instrument include the cardio-sphygmograph, the pneumograph, and the galvanograph. Blood pressure and heart rate are measured by the cardio-sphygmograph component of the polygraph, which consists of a blood pressure cuff that is wrapped around the subject's arm. During the questioning the cuff remains inflated. The movement of blood through the subject's veins generates a sound that is transmitted through the air in the cuff to a bellows that amplifies the sound. The magnitude of the sound relates to the blood pressure and the frequency of the changes in the sound relates to the heart rate. The pneumograph component of the polygraph records the subject's respiratory rate. One tube is placed around the subject's chest and a second is placed around his or her abdomen. These tubes are filled with air. When the subject breaths, changes in the air pressure in the tubes are recorded on the polygraph. The galvanograph section records the amount of perspiration produced. It consists of electrical sensors called galvanometers that are attached to the subject's fingertips. The skin of the fingertips contains a high density of sweat glands, making them a good location to measure perspiration. As the amount of sweat touching the galvanometers increases, the resistance of the electrical current measured decreases and these changes are recorded by the polygraph. Most forensic psychophysiologists (FPs) consider the cardiosphygomgraph and the pneumograph components more informative than the galvanograph.

The Polygraph Test

During the polygraph test, the examiner and the subject are alone in the questioning room. Before the test begins, the examiner spends about an hour talking with the subject. Most forensic psychophisiologists consider this pretest phase an extremely important part of the polygraph. The examiner obtains a baseline read on his or her emotional state and develops the questions that are asked during the actual test. Before the test begins, the examiner goes over each question with the subject so that he or she knows exactly what to expect. When they are ready start, the person administering the polygraph attaches the various components of the polygraph instrument to the examinee.

The polygraph test itself usually consists of about 10 to 12 questions that require yes or no responses. Several methods of composing questions for polygraph tests exist, but all include asking the subject both relevant questions and control questions. Relevant questions relate

directly to the focus of the polygraph test. Examples of relevant questions are "Did you commit crime X?" or "Did you ever use drug Y?" Control questions vary depending on the type of test administered. The most common type of polygraph test is the Control Question Test (CQT), in which control questions are composed so that the subject can answer them honestly, however, the examiner may make them slightly provocative to evoke an emotional response. Examples of control questions are "Did you ever think of doing crime Y?" or "Were you ever drunk in the last year?" This allows the examiner to understand the subject's physiological responses to challenging questions. In the CQT, greater physiological responses to the relevant questions than to the control questions indicate deceptive behavior.

There are variations to the CQT. In Directed Lie Tests (DLT), the examiner substitutes very broad questions for the control questions and the subject is directed to answer them with lies. An example is "Have you ever told a lie?" to which the subject is directed to respond "No." This response gives an examiner an understanding of the subject's physiological response associated with lying. In Positive Control Tests (PCT), a relevant question itself is used as a control. The subject is instructed to answer truthfully the first time the question is asked and falsely the second time it is asked. The only factor that influences the response is whether or not the subject is lying. In the Truth Control Test (TCT), the control questions are composed to make the subject think that he or she is being accused of a fictitious crime. This gives the examiner information on how the subject responds to a truthful denial.

During the post-test, the forensic pschophysiologist analyzes the subject's responses to the questions and scores them. Each channel of the polygraph is scored individually. For any channel, if the control response is larger than the relevant response, the score is from +1 to +3, depending on the magnitude of the difference. If the relevant response is larger the score is from 1 to 3. The scores are summed over all channels and all repetitions of the questions to get to the total score. If the final score is sufficiently large and positive, then the subject is considered to have made truthful statements. If the final score is sufficiently large and negative, then the statements are considered deceptive. If the result is close to zero, then the test is inconclusive.

There is much debate as to the accuracy of polygraph tests. Most forensic psychophysiologists agree that the rate of detecting deceptive behavior is greater than the rate of detecting truthful behavior. The American Polygraph Association claims that the accuracy rate for polygraph tests is between 85 and 95 percent. However, reports of false positives have reached as high as 75 percent in research done by the Congressional Office of Technology Assessment.

History and Uses of the Polygraph

Methods for determining whether or not a person is lying have been part of civilization since ancient times. Ancient Hindus required an accused person to chew a mouthful of rice and then spit it out on a leaf from a sacred tree. If the person could spit the rice he or she was declared honest and if the rice stuck in the mouth, dishonest. This test presumptively relies on the physiological response, which makes a person's mouth dry when being deceptive. In the nineteenth century, Italian criminologist Cesare Lombroso developed an early device for measuring and determining the pulse and blood pressure of a person undergoing interrogation, similar to the cardio-sphymograph component of the polygraph. In the early 1900s, Russian psychologist A. R. Luria measured the reaction time and tremors in the fingers of suspected criminals.

A student in experimental psychology at Harvard University, William M. Marston invented the modern polygraph prior to 1921. His treatise The Lie Detector Test on understanding physiological responses related to deception was published in 1938. John A. Larson, a police officer in Berkeley, California, modified Marston's polygraph, developing a technique for continuous recording of physiological responses. One of Larson's colleagues, Leonarde Keeler, added the gavanograph component to the polygraph. He joined the faculty of Northwestern University School of Law in Chicago in 1930 and established the Keeler Polygraph Institute of Chicago.

Lawyer, John E. Reid played an important role in the development of questioning techniques used during a polygraph test. In a 1947 paper, he described the use of control questions to evoke emotional responses. In collaboration with Cleve Backster's work, this idea eventually became the Control Question Test (CQT), which is used by the majority of forensic psychophysiologists today.

During the 1960s and 1970s, the polygraph business grew rapidly. Employee screening became a multi-million dollar industry. Polygraph testing began to be used routinely in police work and polygraphers were used as expert witnesses in criminal court trials.

During the late 1970s and early 1980s, the use of the polygraph by the military and security agencies expanded drastically. Between 1973 and 1983, polygraph tests by the federal government tripled. By 1985, the Department of Defense was administering 25,000 tests a year. They used polygraphs to screen employees for classified status, for counterintelligence and for criminal investigations. The FBI, CIA, and National Security Agency used the polygraph to screen job candidates. In 1979, two-thirds of the people rejected for employment from CIA jobs were rejected on the basis of failed polygraph tests.

In the 1980s the scientific validity of polygraphs was brought into question by psychologists. In 1988, the federal Polygraph Protection Act was passed, prohibiting employers from using polygraphs for employment screening. As a result of this legislation, businesses can ask an employee to take a polygraph, but the employee's refusal will not result in any disciplinary treatment. This law does not protect government employees, including people who work in schools, prisons, public agencies, and businesses under contract with the federal government.

The use of polygraphs in court was brought to trial in 1989. In the case of United States v. Piccinonna, a polygraph was deemed admissible as evidence, only if both sides agree to its use or the judge allows it based on criteria set forth in the case. A Supreme Court ruling in 1998 expanded the judge's authority in the use of polygraphs in federal cases. Some states accept this ruling, but not all. On the state level, polygraph use is dependent upon the judge and the case. And, in U.S. v. Schellee (1998), the Supreme Court upheld a personal evidentiary rule against the admissibility of polygraph evidence at military trials.

FURTHER READING:

BOOKS:

Harrelson, Leonard. Lietest: Deception, Truth and the Polygraph. Ft. Wayne, IN: Jonas Publishing, 1998.

Lykken, David T. A Tremor in the Blood: Uses and Abuses of the Lie Detector. Reading, MA: Perseus Books, 1998.

Jussim, Daniel. Drug Tests and Polygraphs. New York: Julian Messner, 1987.

ELECTRONIC:

How Stuff Works. "How Lie Detectors Work." <http://science.howstuffworks.com/lie-detector.htm/printable> (April 15, 2003).

American Polygraph Association. <http://www.polygraph.org/> (April 15, 2003).

As an expert in forensic psychophysiology and the use of polygraph tests, I bring a wealth of knowledge and experience to shed light on the concepts discussed in the provided article. My expertise is grounded in a deep understanding of the principles underlying polygraph testing, the components of polygraph instruments, the history of lie detection methods, and the ongoing debates surrounding the accuracy of polygraph tests.

Let's delve into the key concepts presented in the article:

  1. Polygraph Test Basics:

    • A polygraph test is designed to determine the veracity of statements made by a subject.
    • The test monitors physiological responses, including blood pressure, heart rate, respiration rate, and sweat production.
    • The underlying theory is that lying triggers involuntary physiological changes that can be detected by the polygraph instrument.
  2. Polygraph Instrument Components:

    • The polygraph instrument combines three components: cardio-sphygmograph, pneumograph, and galvanograph.
    • The cardio-sphygmograph measures blood pressure and heart rate.
    • The pneumograph records the subject's respiratory rate.
    • The galvanograph measures perspiration levels using electrical sensors on the subject's fingertips.
  3. Polygraph Test Procedure:

    • The test involves a pretest phase where the examiner spends time with the subject to establish a baseline and develop relevant questions.
    • The actual test consists of 10 to 12 yes/no questions, including both relevant and control questions.
    • Control questions are designed to evoke an emotional response and serve as a baseline for comparison.
    • Various types of polygraph tests exist, including the Control Question Test (CQT), Directed Lie Tests (DLT), Positive Control Tests (PCT), and Truth Control Test (TCT).
  4. Scoring and Interpretation:

    • After the test, a forensic psychophysiologist analyzes the subject's physiological responses to each question.
    • Each channel of the polygraph is scored individually, with positive scores indicating truthfulness and negative scores suggesting deception.
    • The total score determines whether the statements are considered truthful, deceptive, or inconclusive.
  5. Accuracy and Controversies:

    • There is ongoing debate about the accuracy of polygraph tests.
    • The American Polygraph Association claims an accuracy rate between 85 and 95 percent, but reports of false positives have been documented.
    • The use of polygraphs has faced scrutiny, leading to the Polygraph Protection Act in 1988, limiting their use in employment screening.
  6. History of Polygraph:

    • Methods for detecting deception date back to ancient times, with various cultures employing physiological indicators.
    • William M. Marston, a student at Harvard, invented the modern polygraph in the early 20th century.
    • The polygraph evolved over the years, with contributions from figures like John A. Larson and Leonarde Keeler.
  7. Legal and Ethical Considerations:

    • The use of polygraphs in legal settings has varied over time, with court rulings shaping their admissibility.
    • The federal Polygraph Protection Act and subsequent legal decisions have influenced the use of polygraphs in employment and legal contexts.

In conclusion, the polygraph test is a complex and controversial tool for detecting deception, involving a combination of physiological measurements and psychological principles. While it has a historical foundation and has undergone technological advancements, its accuracy and reliability remain subjects of ongoing discussion and scrutiny within the field of forensic psychophysiology.

Lie Detector Tests | Encyclopedia.com (2024)

FAQs

How credible are lie detector tests? ›

They estimate the accuracy of the polygraph to be 87%. That is, in 87 out of 100 cases, the polygraph can accurately determine if someone is lying or telling the truth. That sounds pretty impressive, but it is important to keep in mind that the polygraph is failing 13% of the time.

Is lie detector test 100 correct? ›

Martin suggested that when conducted properly, polygraphs are correct 98% of the time, but no scientific evidence has been offered for this. The history of the polygraph is the subject of the documentary film The Lie Detector, which first aired on American Experience on January 3, 2023.

Is there a lie detector that actually works? ›

First created in 1921 by a law enforcement officer, the polygraph machine's sole purpose was to track moment-to-moment physiological changes in a person's blood pressure, heart rate and respiration as proof of deception. Today, we know that polygraphs are not the lie detectors that we trusted them to be.

What disqualifies you on a polygraph? ›

Falsification or minimization in your requested information. Arrests for anything other than minor traffic violations. Commission of a crime which has not been detected. Concealment of anything in your background that would affect your chances for this position.

Can an honest person fail a lie detector test? ›

Unfortunately, it is quite possible to fail a polygraph test even if a person is innocent and telling the truth.

Why did I fail a polygraph when I told the truth? ›

Why did I fail a polygraph and I was telling the truth? Polygraphs are not admissible in court because many things can affect the results. Nerves, health conditions, medications, emotional state - they can all cause false positive or false negative readings. Additionally, subconscious thoughts can have an affect.

Can you pass a lie detector test even if your lying? ›

In essence, it is within the realms of possibility to pass a lie detector test when you are lying but it's very unlikely – the best advice is not to rely on so called strategies and techniques you have learned from the internet or a helpful mate but to just not take the test if you intend to lie.

Can an innocent person fail a polygraph test? ›

Even if you are innocent and have never committed any crime, your polygraph test results can come back negative, making it look like you were lying. Innocent people can fail lie detector tests because of their physiological responses to the test questions.

Can you outsmart a lie detector test? ›

Polygraph testing is easily gamed by countermeasures like controlling your breathing, anxiety, using pain, etc., and even by being nice to the person doing the testing (see Wikipedia, "Polygraph").

What is the easiest way to detect a lie? ›

Watch for inappropriate, unusual, or uncommon behavior.

Also watch for common liars' mistakes like mismatching words and body language. They might say “no” while nodding “yes.” They could exhibit strange emotions (laughing when the subject is serious, for example).

What is the best home lie detector test? ›

Best Lie Detector: EyeDetect | 97-99% Accuracy with Polygraph. Combine EyeDetect with Polygraph to get 97-99% outcome confidence. The two combined are the best lie detector test: highest accuracy.

Can anxiety affect a lie detector test? ›

According to a report from the National Academy of Sciences, “[a] variety of mental and physical factors, such as anxiety about being tested, can affect polygraph results – making the technique susceptible to error.” Unfortunately, once you have failed a government polygraph test, there may be little you can do to ...

What can throw off a polygraph test? ›

Factors such as medications, illness, and even simple things like caffeine can all affect the outcome of the test. It's important to understand how these factors can influence the results of a polygraph test, especially if you're going to be taking one for an important reason.

Can you pass a lie detector test if you have anxiety? ›

Yes, mental illness can affect the results. As mentioned, certain ones, such as anxiety disorders, can cause physiological responses that may be interpreted as signs of deception. It is important to disclose any mental health conditions or medications to the examiner before the test.

What is more accurate than a lie detector test? ›

fMRI scans revealed activation of decision-making areas of the brain. Experts examining fMRI scans were 24 percent more likely to spot a lie. This is compared with the results of professional polygraph examiners.

What are the odds of failing a lie detector test? ›

The failure rate is difficult to determine, as it can vary depending on a number of factors, including the specific test being administered, the population being tested, and the skill of the test administrator. However, some studies have suggested that it can be as high as 20%.

What are the chances of passing a lie detector test? ›

Lie detector testing is generally around 87.5% accurate and there are instances where the outcome is inconclusive or where human error occurs (as in every human endeavour) or where technological failure happens.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Horacio Brakus JD

Last Updated:

Views: 5867

Rating: 4 / 5 (51 voted)

Reviews: 90% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Horacio Brakus JD

Birthday: 1999-08-21

Address: Apt. 524 43384 Minnie Prairie, South Edda, MA 62804

Phone: +5931039998219

Job: Sales Strategist

Hobby: Sculling, Kitesurfing, Orienteering, Painting, Computer programming, Creative writing, Scuba diving

Introduction: My name is Horacio Brakus JD, I am a lively, splendid, jolly, vivacious, vast, cheerful, agreeable person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.