In Vino Veritas? Alcohol, Response Inhibition and Lying (2024)

Article Navigation

Article Contents

  • INTRODUCTION

  • METHOD

  • RESULTS

  • DISCUSSION

  • SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

  • Funding

  • Conflict of interest statement

  • Acknowledgements

  • REFERENCES

Journal Article

,

Kristina Suchotzki

1

Department of

Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology

,

Ghent University

,

Henri Dunantlaan 2, 9000 Ghent

,

Belgium

*Corresponding author: Department of Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Henri Dunantlaan 2, B-9000 Ghent, Belgium. Tel.: +32-9-264-94-46; Fax: +

32-9-264-64-89

; E-mail: kristina.suchotzki@ugent.be

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Geert Crombez

1

Department of

Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology

,

Ghent University

,

Henri Dunantlaan 2, 9000 Ghent

,

Belgium

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Evelyne Debey

1

Department of

Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology

,

Ghent University

,

Henri Dunantlaan 2, 9000 Ghent

,

Belgium

Search for other works by this author on:

,

Kim van Oorsouw

2

Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience

,

Maastricht University

,

Universiteitssingel 40, 6229 ER Maastricht

,

The Netherlands

Search for other works by this author on:

Bruno Verschuere

1

Department of

Experimental Clinical and Health Psychology

,

Ghent University

,

Henri Dunantlaan 2, 9000 Ghent

,

Belgium

2

Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience

,

Maastricht University

,

Universiteitssingel 40, 6229 ER Maastricht

,

The Netherlands

3

Department of Clinical Psychology

,

University of Amsterdam

,

Weesperplein 4, 1018 XA Amsterdam

,

The Netherlands

Search for other works by this author on:

Alcohol and Alcoholism, Volume 50, Issue 1, January/February 2015, Pages 74–81, https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agu079

Published:

14 November 2014

Article history

Received:

16 July 2014

Revision requested:

22 September 2014

Revision received:

10 October 2014

Accepted:

17 October 2014

Published:

14 November 2014

  • In Vino Veritas? Alcohol, Response Inhibition and Lying (2)PDF
  • Split View
  • Views
    • Article contents
    • Figures & tables
    • Video
    • Audio
    • Supplementary Data
  • Cite

    Cite

    Kristina Suchotzki, Geert Crombez, Evelyne Debey, Kim van Oorsouw, Bruno Verschuere, In Vino Veritas? Alcohol, Response Inhibition and Lying, Alcohol and Alcoholism, Volume 50, Issue 1, January/February 2015, Pages 74–81, https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agu079

    Close

Search

Close

Search

Search Menu

Aims: Despite the widespread belief that alcohol makes the truth come out more easily, we know very little on how alcohol impacts deception. Given that alcohol impairs response inhibition, and that response inhibition may be critically involved in deception, we expected that alcohol intake would hamper lying. Methods: In total, 104 volunteers were tested at a science festival, where they had the opportunity to drink alcohol. Stop-Signal Reaction Times (SSRTs) served as operationalization of response inhibition. Differences in error rates and reaction times (RTs) between lying and truth telling served as indicators of the cognitive cost of lying. Results: Higher blood alcohol concentration was related to longer SSRTs, but unrelated to the cognitive costs of lying. Conclusion: This study validates previous laboratory research on alcohol and response inhibition in a realistic drinking environment, yet failed to find an effect of alcohol on lying. Implications of these findings and for the role of response inhibition in lying are discussed.

INTRODUCTION

In vino veritas’, ‘Drunks and children always speak the truth’ and ‘Alcohol loosens the tongue’ are only some expressions of the widespread belief that alcohol makes the truth come out more easily. Yet, there is nearly no research on the relationship between alcohol and lying, which is unexpected considering the substantial number of crimes committed by intoxicated offenders (Sigurdsson and Gudjonsson, 1994; Haggard-Grann et al., 2006).

Theoretical support for the hypothesis that alcohol may hamper lying comes from research showing that alcohol hampers response inhibition. Response inhibition is most often defined as the intentional suppression of dominant, automatic or prepotent responses (Miyake et al., 2000). Experimental laboratory studies have shown that moderate blood alcohol concentration (BAC; 0.04–0.08%) can impair performance in behavioral measures of response inhibition, such as the Stop-Signal task or the Go/No-Go task (Mulvihill et al., 1997; Fillmore and Vogel-Sprott, 1999, 2000; de Wit et al., 2000; Marczinski and Fillmore, 2003; Fillmore et al., 2009; Anderson et al., 2011; Tsujii et al., 2011; Nikolaou et al., 2013; for a review see Fillmore, 2007). Crucially, lying almost by definition involves the inhibition of the truth response. Prolonged reaction times (RTs) and an increased error rate (ER) for lying compared with truth telling have been interpreted as a cognitive cost of the conflict between the prepotent truth response and the deceptive response (Walczyk et al., 2003; Spence et al., 2008; Seymour and Schumacher, 2009; Verschuere and De Houwer, 2011). This claim has been further supported by research showing that lying is accompanied by increased activation in brain regions that are crucially involved in response inhibition tasks (e.g. the right inferior frontal gyrus; Spence et al., 2001, 2008; Aron et al., 2004, 2014; Christ et al., 2009; Gamer, 2011; Vartanian et al., 2013). As there are indications that the effect of alcohol on response inhibition might be mediated by the depressing effects of alcohol on neural activity in the right inferior frontal cortex (Tsujii et al., 2011), one might hypothesize that alcohol intake not only interferes with response inhibition, but also with lying.

A contrasting prediction, namely that alcohol intake improves deception, can be derived from the findings of Karim et al. (2010). Inhibiting neuronal activity in the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC), a region that has previously been linked to moral cognition (Greene et al., 2001; Moll et al., 2002, 2005), facilitated lying as evidenced by shorter RTs and decreased skin-conductance responses. The authors also observed diminished feelings of guilt to deceive the interrogator after aPFC inhibition and proposed that the facilitation may be caused by a diminished experience of moral conflict. Alcohol impacts on multiple brain areas and has been observed to disinhibit ‘immoral’ behavior under certain conditions (Bond, 1998; Lyvers, 2000; Leeman et al., 2009), and could therefore also facilitate lying.

There are only a few studies that investigated the impact of alcohol in a lie detection context. Bradley and Ainsworth (1984) studied the effects of alcohol intake on the psychophysiological detection of crime-related information. Alcohol intoxication (BACs around 0.12%) during a polygraph examination did not affect detection accuracy, but intoxication during a preceding mock crime decreased crime memory detection. Yet, O'Toole et al. (1994) were unable to replicate the latter finding. These two studies were the first to investigate the influence of alcohol in a forensic ‘lie detection’ context, but they speak more to the effect of alcohol on memory. More relevant for deception is a study by Kireev et al. (2008), in which participants performed the same deception paradigm twice, once sober and once after alcohol intake. In their paradigm, participants freely chose on each trial whether to respond truthfully or deceitfully (i.e. to indicate with one of two buttons correctly or incorrectly the directions of simple arrows) with the purpose to ‘deceive’ a computer. Results were mixed. RTs for lying were significantly longer than for truth telling in the sober condition, whereas this difference was not significant in the alcohol condition. Yet, neither RTs for truth telling nor RTs for lying differed significantly between the sober and the alcohol condition, and statistical information regarding the crucial interaction between lie/truth and intoxicated/sober was not reported. Using an event-related potential (ERP) measure, Kireev et al. (2008) also found a larger N190 for lying compared with truth telling in the sober condition, but a reversed N190 effect in the alcohol condition. As the N190 is regarded as related to error perception (‘error-related negativity’), this finding was taken as an indication that sober participants, but not intoxicated participants, perceived lying as an ‘error’. These results fit with the results and the interpretation that alcohol may improve lying by reducing moral conflict (Karim et al., 2010), but should be treated with caution. Although Kireev et al. (2008) compared a sober with an intoxicated condition, they did not find significant BAC differences between both conditions and did not report the respective mean BACs. Furthermore, the sample size was small (n = 13) and participants could freely choose between truth telling and lying so that there was no possibility to differentiate between intentional lies and behavioral errors.

The goal of the present study was to investigate the relationship between alcohol, response inhibition and lying. To that means, we chose a real-life drinking situation that enabled us to test a large number of volunteers with varying blood alcohol levels. The study therefore aimed not only to elucidate the relationship between alcohol and lying, but also to add to the alcohol and response inhibition literature by investigating in a large sample whether the effects of controlled alcohol intake in laboratory settings generalize to real-life drinking environments, in which participants freely determine their drinking behavior. Response inhibition in our study was measured as the estimated time of stopping a prepotent go-response (SSRT) in the Stop-Signal Task (Vince, 1948; Lappin and Eriksen, 1966; Logan and Cowan, 1984). Lying was measured with the Sheffield Lie Test (Spence et al., 2001; based on the Differentiation of Deception paradigm, Furedy et al., 1988). In this paradigm, one typically observes an enhanced ER and prolonged RTs for lying compared with truth telling. These lie effects (ERlying − ERtruth telling; RTlying − RTtruth telling) were taken as indication of the cognitive cost of lying (Spence et al., 2001; Fullam et al., 2009; Farrow et al., 2010; Verschuere et al., 2011; Debey et al., 2012; Hu et al., 2012; Van Bockstaele et al., 2012). Based on previous laboratory research showing that alcohol impairs response inhibition, we expected higher BACs to be related to longer SSRTs. Based on previous research showing that lying comes at a cognitive cost, we expected to replicate both lie effects (in ER and RTs). Based on the research that implies a crucial role of response inhibition in deception, we expected higher BACs to be related to an increased cognitive cost of lying (i.e. larger ER and RT lie effects). As also habitual alcohol use was found to be associated with impairments in stop-signal performance (Nigg et al., 2006; Lawrence et al., 2009), we included an assessment of problematic drinking behavior (AUDIT). Considering the substantial overlap of the concepts of response inhibition and impulsivity as well as findings that increased impulsivity is implicated in the development and maintenance of substance abuse disorders (de Wit, 2009), we also included a measurement of trait impulsivity (BIS-11).

METHOD

Participants

In total, 104 visitors of the science festival Discovery Day 2012 volunteered to participate in the study. The study was approved by the ethical committee of Maastricht University and all participants provided written informed consent. Data of participants were excluded from data-analyses when participants had reported drug and/or medication use (n = 14). Furthermore, we excluded data of participants that exceeded the mean ER plus 2.5 standard deviations in the Stop-Signal Task or the Sheffield Lie Test (n = 2). The mean age and gender of the remaining 88 participants can be found in Table 2.

Procedure

Testing took place at two locations of the festival (Rotterdam and Amsterdam) from 9.00 PM to 3.00 AM. The study was advertised as investigating the relation between alcohol and lying, and had been announced on national radio earlier that day. Following the advice of the ethical committee, everyone interested in the study could participate and participants were not selected on the basis of their alcohol consumption. Participants were not encouraged to drink alcohol.

Participants filled out a questionnaire assessing demographic variables (gender and age), feelings of tension, anxiety, intoxication, tiredness and concentration (1–10 Likert scales), drinking behavior on that day (number of alcoholic consumptions and drinking time) and drugs or medication use on that day. Trait impulsivity was assessed with the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11; Patton and Stanford, 1995) and habitual alcohol use was assessed with the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT). Testing took place on four computers, which allowed simultaneous testing of four participants. In each location, three experimenters conducted the study. For every participant, the time of testing was noted in order to control for it in statistical analyses as a potential confound. Participants were not allowed to drink during the experiment to ensure a minimum of 15 min (i.e. the duration of both tasks) between the last alcoholic drink and the alcohol test. Everyone first executed the Stop-Signal Task and then the Sheffield Lie Test. Finally, participants were asked to drink a sip of water and BAC was measured with the Dräger Alcotest 6510. The Dräger Alcotest 6510 converts the breath alcohol ratio into blood alcohol concentration (BAC in %). Finally, participants were told their BAC values. If participants were severely intoxicated, they were warned about the consequences of severe alcohol intake and they were advised to stop drinking. Participants were thoroughly debriefed about the purpose and the background of the experiment and received a handout with information and contact details of the experimenter in case they had any further questions.

Stop-signal task

The Stop-Signal Task was programmed and presented with Tscope, a C/C++ library (Stevens et al., 2006). During the task, two types of stimuli (an ‘X’ or ‘O’) were presented in white in the center of a black screen. Participants were instructed to indicate with left and right button presses which of the two stimuli they saw (‘z’ (left) and ‘/’ (right) on a standard QWERTY keyboard). Stimuli and response mappings were counterbalanced across participants. The response deadline was 2000 ms and the inter-trial interval was 300 ms. On 75% of the trials, participants simply had to perform the binary decision as fast as possible (go-trials). Crucially, on 25% of the trials, a signal (a 1000 Hz tone) was presented for 100 ms via a headphone, indicating that participants should try to stop their response. The time interval between the stimulus and the stop-signal (stop-signal delay, or SSD) was initially set to 250 ms, but adjusted on a trial-to-trial basis. After a successful stop, it was increased by 50 ms, after a failure to stop it was decreased by 50 ms. The test phase consisted of two blocks of 80 trials, with 20 stop trials each (160 trials in total, including 40 stop trials). Test blocks were separated by a self-paced break. As a measure of response inhibition, we calculated the SSRT by subtracting the mean SSD from the mean RT on go-signal trials (Verbruggen et al., 2008). The SSRT is a well-validated measure of response inhibition ability (for reviews see Logan, 1994; Boucher et al., 2007; Verbruggen and Logan, 2008).

Before the actual test, participants practiced the task. In a first practice phase, consisting of eight trials, participants practiced the go-response while ignoring the stop-signals. In a second phase, consisting of 16 trials, participants practiced to inhibit their response on 4 stop-signal trials.

Sheffield lie test

The Sheffield Lie Test was presented with Inquisit 3.0.1. In the Sheffield Lie Test, participants have to answer Yes/No questions both truthfully and deceptively, depending on a color cue. Thirty questions (15 with ‘yes’ and 15 with ‘no’ as correct response) were presented verbally via headphones, in random order. For example: ‘Is Amsterdam in the Netherlands?’, ‘Is Amsterdam in Switzerland?’. All questions can be found in Table 1. Each question was presented four times, and had to be answered twice truthfully and twice deceptively. Reminder labels for the possible responses (‘Yes’/‘No’) appeared on the left and right lower part of a black screen and responses had to be given with left and right button presses (‘z’ (left) and ‘/’ (right) on a standard QWERTY keyboard). The response labels were presented in yellow or blue, and participants were instructed that one color required a truthful response, whereas the other required a lie. The position of the reminder labels and color-assignment were counterbalanced across participants. Participants were instructed to respond as fast as possible. If participants did not respond after 6000 ms, the labels disappeared and the words ‘Too slow’ were presented centrally on the screen. The inter-trial interval was 200 ms. The test phase consisted of two blocks, with 60 trials each (120 trials in total, including 60 truth and 60 lie trials). Test blocks were separated by a self-paced break. As measure of the cognitive costs of lying, we calculated the ER and RT lie effects by subtracting the mean of the truth telling condition from the mean of the lying condition (ERlying − ERtruth telling; RTlying − RTtruth telling).

Table 1.

Questions used in the Sheffield Lie Test (translated from Dutch)

Questions requiring ‘yes’ as correct responseQuestions requiring ‘no’ as correct response
Is water wet?Is water dry?
Is ice cold?Is ice warm?
Can birds fly?Can pigs fly?
Is a crocodile an animal?Is a computer an animal?
Is Amsterdam in the Netherlands?Is Amsterdam in Switzerland?
Are giants big?Are giants small?
Do cars have four wheels?Do cars have six wheels?
Is an igloo made of ice?Is an igloo made of stone?
Is sausage meat?Is salad meat?
Is stone hard?Is stone soft?
Is fire warm?Is fire wet?
Is milk white?Is milk green?
Are bananas yellow?Are bananas red?
Is grass green?Is grass blue?
Does a butcher sell meat?Does a butcher sell bread?
Questions requiring ‘yes’ as correct responseQuestions requiring ‘no’ as correct response
Is water wet?Is water dry?
Is ice cold?Is ice warm?
Can birds fly?Can pigs fly?
Is a crocodile an animal?Is a computer an animal?
Is Amsterdam in the Netherlands?Is Amsterdam in Switzerland?
Are giants big?Are giants small?
Do cars have four wheels?Do cars have six wheels?
Is an igloo made of ice?Is an igloo made of stone?
Is sausage meat?Is salad meat?
Is stone hard?Is stone soft?
Is fire warm?Is fire wet?
Is milk white?Is milk green?
Are bananas yellow?Are bananas red?
Is grass green?Is grass blue?
Does a butcher sell meat?Does a butcher sell bread?

Table 1.

Questions used in the Sheffield Lie Test (translated from Dutch)

Questions requiring ‘yes’ as correct responseQuestions requiring ‘no’ as correct response
Is water wet?Is water dry?
Is ice cold?Is ice warm?
Can birds fly?Can pigs fly?
Is a crocodile an animal?Is a computer an animal?
Is Amsterdam in the Netherlands?Is Amsterdam in Switzerland?
Are giants big?Are giants small?
Do cars have four wheels?Do cars have six wheels?
Is an igloo made of ice?Is an igloo made of stone?
Is sausage meat?Is salad meat?
Is stone hard?Is stone soft?
Is fire warm?Is fire wet?
Is milk white?Is milk green?
Are bananas yellow?Are bananas red?
Is grass green?Is grass blue?
Does a butcher sell meat?Does a butcher sell bread?
Questions requiring ‘yes’ as correct responseQuestions requiring ‘no’ as correct response
Is water wet?Is water dry?
Is ice cold?Is ice warm?
Can birds fly?Can pigs fly?
Is a crocodile an animal?Is a computer an animal?
Is Amsterdam in the Netherlands?Is Amsterdam in Switzerland?
Are giants big?Are giants small?
Do cars have four wheels?Do cars have six wheels?
Is an igloo made of ice?Is an igloo made of stone?
Is sausage meat?Is salad meat?
Is stone hard?Is stone soft?
Is fire warm?Is fire wet?
Is milk white?Is milk green?
Are bananas yellow?Are bananas red?
Is grass green?Is grass blue?
Does a butcher sell meat?Does a butcher sell bread?

Before the actual test, participants practiced the task with 12 different questions. Only during the practice phase, participants received additional feedback after each trial on the correctness of their response.

RESULTS

Descriptives

As can be seen in Fig. 1, the distribution of BAC was positively skewed with an overrepresentation of BAC = 0.00% (n = 31; zskewness = 5.93, P < 0.001; zkurtosis = 4.30, P < 0.001). BACs ranged between 0.00 and 0.15%, with an average BAC of 0.03% (SD = 0.03; Mdn = 0.02). Means and standard deviations of all other assessed variables can be found in Table 2.

Table 2.

Means, standard deviations and correlations (rs) with BAC

MeasureMSDBAC
BAC0.030.03
Gender0.500.50−0.14
Age28.026.240.05
Time of testing230.75105.200.61***
Tension3.162.14−0.03
Anxiety1.620.80−0.12
Tiredness5.052.02−0.10
Concentration5.382.06−0.11
BIS-1153.959.160.11
AUDIT9.734.800.53***
Manipulation checks
 Intoxication3.512.220.74***
 No. consumptions3.602.920.81***
 Drinking time2.772.400.75***
MeasureMSDBAC
BAC0.030.03
Gender0.500.50−0.14
Age28.026.240.05
Time of testing230.75105.200.61***
Tension3.162.14−0.03
Anxiety1.620.80−0.12
Tiredness5.052.02−0.10
Concentration5.382.06−0.11
BIS-1153.959.160.11
AUDIT9.734.800.53***
Manipulation checks
 Intoxication3.512.220.74***
 No. consumptions3.602.920.81***
 Drinking time2.772.400.75***

Gender = % female; Time of testing = Time of testing in minutes after 20 h; No. consumptions = Number of alcohol consumptions; Drinking time = Drinking time in hours. P-values reported two-tailed.

***

P < 0.001.

Table 2.

Means, standard deviations and correlations (rs) with BAC

MeasureMSDBAC
BAC0.030.03
Gender0.500.50−0.14
Age28.026.240.05
Time of testing230.75105.200.61***
Tension3.162.14−0.03
Anxiety1.620.80−0.12
Tiredness5.052.02−0.10
Concentration5.382.06−0.11
BIS-1153.959.160.11
AUDIT9.734.800.53***
Manipulation checks
 Intoxication3.512.220.74***
 No. consumptions3.602.920.81***
 Drinking time2.772.400.75***
MeasureMSDBAC
BAC0.030.03
Gender0.500.50−0.14
Age28.026.240.05
Time of testing230.75105.200.61***
Tension3.162.14−0.03
Anxiety1.620.80−0.12
Tiredness5.052.02−0.10
Concentration5.382.06−0.11
BIS-1153.959.160.11
AUDIT9.734.800.53***
Manipulation checks
 Intoxication3.512.220.74***
 No. consumptions3.602.920.81***
 Drinking time2.772.400.75***

Gender = % female; Time of testing = Time of testing in minutes after 20 h; No. consumptions = Number of alcohol consumptions; Drinking time = Drinking time in hours. P-values reported two-tailed.

***

P < 0.001.

Fig. 1.

Distribution of the blood alcohol concentration (in %) in our sample (n = 88).

Preliminary analysis and manipulation check

Paired sample t-tests confirmed that lying (M = 10.34%, SD = 7.43) was associated with a higher ER than truth telling (M = 6.79%, SD = 5.45), t(87) = 5.48, P < 0.001, d = 0.58. [For group comparisons, the standardized mean difference d was calculated as measure of effect size, with 0.20, 0.50 and 0.80 as thresholds for ‘small’, ‘moderate’ and ‘large’ effects (Cohen, 1988). When computing d for dependent samples, we corrected d for inter-correlations (Dunlap et al., 1996; Morris and DeShon, 2002)] After removal of error trials and RT outliers (0.02%; RTs >2.5 SDs from the mean per subject and condition), paired sample t-tests confirmed that lying (M = 3315 ms, SD = 326) was associated with longer RTs compared with truth telling (M = 3149 ms, SD = 293), t(87) = 9.19, P < 0.001, d = 0.98. Means and standard deviations of SSRT, ER lie effect and RT lie effect can be found in Table 3.

Table 3.

Means, standard deviations and intercorrelations (rs) of BAC, dependent variables, time of testing, BIS-11, and AUDIT

MeasureMSDBACSSRTER lie effectRT lie effectTime of testingBIS-11AUDIT
BAC0.030.03
SSRT296.29143.150.35**
ER lie effect3.556.080.070.02
RT lie effect166.51170.040.080.040.11
Time of testing230.75105.200.61***0.34**0.100.11
BIS-1153.959.160.110.070.120.010.12
AUDIT9.734.800.53***0.24*0.17−0.030.27*0.11
MeasureMSDBACSSRTER lie effectRT lie effectTime of testingBIS-11AUDIT
BAC0.030.03
SSRT296.29143.150.35**
ER lie effect3.556.080.070.02
RT lie effect166.51170.040.080.040.11
Time of testing230.75105.200.61***0.34**0.100.11
BIS-1153.959.160.110.070.120.010.12
AUDIT9.734.800.53***0.24*0.17−0.030.27*0.11

Time of testing = Time of testing in minutes after 20 h. P-values reported two-tailed.

*

P < 0.05.

**

P < 0.01.

***

P < 0.001.

Table 3.

Means, standard deviations and intercorrelations (rs) of BAC, dependent variables, time of testing, BIS-11, and AUDIT

MeasureMSDBACSSRTER lie effectRT lie effectTime of testingBIS-11AUDIT
BAC0.030.03
SSRT296.29143.150.35**
ER lie effect3.556.080.070.02
RT lie effect166.51170.040.080.040.11
Time of testing230.75105.200.61***0.34**0.100.11
BIS-1153.959.160.110.070.120.010.12
AUDIT9.734.800.53***0.24*0.17−0.030.27*0.11
MeasureMSDBACSSRTER lie effectRT lie effectTime of testingBIS-11AUDIT
BAC0.030.03
SSRT296.29143.150.35**
ER lie effect3.556.080.070.02
RT lie effect166.51170.040.080.040.11
Time of testing230.75105.200.61***0.34**0.100.11
BIS-1153.959.160.110.070.120.010.12
AUDIT9.734.800.53***0.24*0.17−0.030.27*0.11

Time of testing = Time of testing in minutes after 20 h. P-values reported two-tailed.

*

P < 0.05.

**

P < 0.01.

***

P < 0.001.

As manipulation check, we computed the correlation between BAC and the feeling of intoxication, the number of alcohol consumptions and the drinking time. Because BAC was not normally distributed, we used Spearman's rho (rs) as correlation coefficient in all further analyses. Note that rs also serves as effect size, with 0.10, 0.30 and 0.50 as thresholds for ‘small’, ‘moderate’ and ‘large’ effects. As can be seen in Table 2, higher levels of BAC were related to a higher feeling of intoxication, a larger number of reported alcoholic consumptions and a longer drinking time.

To discriminate between effects of acute alcohol consumption, impulsivity and habitual alcohol use, and to check for possible other confounding variables, we also computed the correlations between BAC and gender, age, time of testing, feelings of tension, anxiety, tiredness, and concentration, the BIS-11, and the AUDIT. As can be seen in Table 2, higher levels of BAC were related to a later time of testing and a stronger habitual alcohol use. We will therefore control for these factors in our dimensional analyses.

Dimensional analyses

To investigate the link between BAC, response inhibition and the cognitive cost of lying, we computed the correlations between BAC, SSRT, ER and RT lie effect. As can be seen in Table 3, higher levels of BAC were related to higher SSRTs, whereas the correlations with the lie effects were not significant.

To control for the influence of the time of testing on the SSRT scores, we checked whether the time of testing was correlated with any of the feelings during the testing and computed the nonparametric partial correlation between BAC and SSRT. Results revealed that time of testing was only related to the feeling of intoxication, rs = 0.58, P < 0.001, but not to any other feeling, all Ps > 0.15. The BAC-SSRT relation was still marginally significant after controlling for the time of testing, r = 0.20, P = 0.07. As higher SSRT scores were related not only to higher BAC levels but also to higher AUDIT scores, we also computed the nonparametric partial correlation between BAC and SSRT to examine whether acute alcohol effects (BAC) were carried by effects of habitual alcohol use (AUDIT). The BAC-SSRT relation remained marginally significant after controlling for the AUDIT scores, r = 0.21, P = 0.07. A multiple linear regression analysis with BAC predicting SSRT also revealed no significant increase in the prediction when adding AUDIT and BAC × AUDIT to the model. Intercorrelations of all assessed variables can be found in Supplementary Data of the online Supplementary Data.

Categorical analyses

To enable a better comparison of our results with previous research that compared groups of participants that received different doses of alcohol with sober controls, we categorized participants according to their BAC levels. As previous research found effects of alcohol on response inhibition from 0.04% on (Mulvihill et al., 1997; Fillmore and Vogel-Sprott, 1999, 2000; de Wit et al., 2000; Marczinski and Fillmore, 2003; Fillmore, 2007; Fillmore et al., 2009; Anderson et al., 2011; Tsujii et al., 2011; Nikolaou et al., 2013), participants with an alcohol level below 0.04% were categorized as sober controls (n = 60), whereas participants with an alcohol level of 0.04% and above were categorized as intoxicated (n = 28).

As can be seen in Table 4, independent-sample t-tests revealed a significantly longer SSRT for the intoxicated group compared with the sober control group, t(34.07) = 2.70, P < 0.05, d = 0.76. There were no significant group differences in the ER lie effect, t(86) = 0.83, P = 0.41, d = 0.19, or the RT lie effect t(86) = 1.13, P = 0.26, d = 0.26.

Table 4.

Means and standard deviations of different variables for the sober and the intoxicated group and results of the independent t-tests

MeasureSoberIntoxicatedtdfP
MSDMSD
BAC0.010.010.070.039.8230.78<0.001
Gender550.50390.501.89a1a0.17a
Age28.637.1926.703.041.7483.660.09
Time of testing203.0597.01290.1098.673.9086<0.001
Tension3.081.933.322.570.48860.63
Anxiety1.730.891.380.502.2276.76<0.05
Tiredness5.221.964.692.151.09790.28
Concentration5.512.125.121.950.80790.43
BIS-1153.328.7155.4010.160.95800.35
AUDIT8.694.7312.353.963.2879<0.01
Manipulation checks
 Intoxication2.531.715.581.707.5179<0.001
 No. consumptions2.262.216.382.177.8778<0.001
 Drinking time2.002.374.381.535.4571.41<0.001
Dependent measures
 SSRT262.9999.21366.47191.312.7034.07<0.05
 ER lie effect3.185.814.346.660.83860.41
 RT lie effect152.54168.56196.47172.391.13860.26
MeasureSoberIntoxicatedtdfP
MSDMSD
BAC0.010.010.070.039.8230.78<0.001
Gender550.50390.501.89a1a0.17a
Age28.637.1926.703.041.7483.660.09
Time of testing203.0597.01290.1098.673.9086<0.001
Tension3.081.933.322.570.48860.63
Anxiety1.730.891.380.502.2276.76<0.05
Tiredness5.221.964.692.151.09790.28
Concentration5.512.125.121.950.80790.43
BIS-1153.328.7155.4010.160.95800.35
AUDIT8.694.7312.353.963.2879<0.01
Manipulation checks
 Intoxication2.531.715.581.707.5179<0.001
 No. consumptions2.262.216.382.177.8778<0.001
 Drinking time2.002.374.381.535.4571.41<0.001
Dependent measures
 SSRT262.9999.21366.47191.312.7034.07<0.05
 ER lie effect3.185.814.346.660.83860.41
 RT lie effect152.54168.56196.47172.391.13860.26

Gender = % female; Time of testing = Time of testing in minutes after 20 h; No. Consumptions = Number of alcohol consumptions. Drinking time = Drinking time in hours. Degrees of freedom are corrected when equality of variances was rejected and differ between measures due to missing values. P-values reported two-tailed.

aAs gender is a categorical variable, Pearson's chi-squared (χ2) test was used.

Table 4.

Means and standard deviations of different variables for the sober and the intoxicated group and results of the independent t-tests

MeasureSoberIntoxicatedtdfP
MSDMSD
BAC0.010.010.070.039.8230.78<0.001
Gender550.50390.501.89a1a0.17a
Age28.637.1926.703.041.7483.660.09
Time of testing203.0597.01290.1098.673.9086<0.001
Tension3.081.933.322.570.48860.63
Anxiety1.730.891.380.502.2276.76<0.05
Tiredness5.221.964.692.151.09790.28
Concentration5.512.125.121.950.80790.43
BIS-1153.328.7155.4010.160.95800.35
AUDIT8.694.7312.353.963.2879<0.01
Manipulation checks
 Intoxication2.531.715.581.707.5179<0.001
 No. consumptions2.262.216.382.177.8778<0.001
 Drinking time2.002.374.381.535.4571.41<0.001
Dependent measures
 SSRT262.9999.21366.47191.312.7034.07<0.05
 ER lie effect3.185.814.346.660.83860.41
 RT lie effect152.54168.56196.47172.391.13860.26
MeasureSoberIntoxicatedtdfP
MSDMSD
BAC0.010.010.070.039.8230.78<0.001
Gender550.50390.501.89a1a0.17a
Age28.637.1926.703.041.7483.660.09
Time of testing203.0597.01290.1098.673.9086<0.001
Tension3.081.933.322.570.48860.63
Anxiety1.730.891.380.502.2276.76<0.05
Tiredness5.221.964.692.151.09790.28
Concentration5.512.125.121.950.80790.43
BIS-1153.328.7155.4010.160.95800.35
AUDIT8.694.7312.353.963.2879<0.01
Manipulation checks
 Intoxication2.531.715.581.707.5179<0.001
 No. consumptions2.262.216.382.177.8778<0.001
 Drinking time2.002.374.381.535.4571.41<0.001
Dependent measures
 SSRT262.9999.21366.47191.312.7034.07<0.05
 ER lie effect3.185.814.346.660.83860.41
 RT lie effect152.54168.56196.47172.391.13860.26

Gender = % female; Time of testing = Time of testing in minutes after 20 h; No. Consumptions = Number of alcohol consumptions. Drinking time = Drinking time in hours. Degrees of freedom are corrected when equality of variances was rejected and differ between measures due to missing values. P-values reported two-tailed.

aAs gender is a categorical variable, Pearson's chi-squared (χ2) test was used.

DISCUSSION

In order to investigate the relation between alcohol consumption, response inhibition and lying, the current study was conducted at a science festival where visitors voluntarily consumed alcohol. Such a naturalistic setting comes at the cost of experimental control, but it enabled us to recruit a large number of volunteers with varying blood alcohol levels, without actively administering alcohol to participants or encouraging alcohol consumption. Furthermore, our study complemented and extended previous laboratory research by demonstrating the generalization of alcohol and lie-effects to more realistic samples and settings.

Results of both the dimensional and the categorical analyses revealed that alcohol intake was associated with impaired response inhibition. Our findings thereby validate previous laboratory research that found impaired response inhibition performances after alcohol intake (Mulvihill et al., 1997; Fillmore and Vogel-Sprott, 1999, 2000; de Wit et al., 2000; Nikolaou et al., 2013). This is important as alcohol intake in a laboratory environment differs from realistic drinking environments in many aspects (e.g. instructed vs. spontaneous alcohol consumption, different environmental cues, social factors and reinforcing effects of alcohol intake). Accordingly, a meta-analysis revealed that both pharmacological and expectancy effects of alcohol intake were significantly moderated by the experimental setting (experimental vs. natural vs. bar setting; McKay and Schare, 1999), stressing the need for demonstrations of experimental effects in more realistic environments. The finding that alcohol effects on response inhibition transfer to realistic drinking environments is also relevant for forensic and clinical contexts, as impaired response inhibition has been linked to aggressive behavior and psychological disorders, such as anti-social personality, obsessive-compulsive and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorders (ADHD; Schachar and Logan, 1990; Schachar et al., 1993; Oosterlaan and Sergeant, 1996; Pawliczek et al., 2013).

In line with theories stating that habitual alcohol use is related to poor response inhibition capacities, either by facilitating the development of a dependency or as consequence of long alcohol abuse (Jentsch and Taylor, 1999; Nigg et al., 2006; de Wit, 2009; Lawrence et al., 2009; Courtney et al., 2013), we also found that stronger habitual alcohol use was related to a worse performance in the Stop-Signal task. Our design does not allow to disentangle acute alcohol effects and habitual alcohol use. Yet, the observation that the correlation between BAC and SSRT was still marginally significant when controlling for the AUDIT scores indicates that the observed response inhibition impairments cannot fully be attributed to habitual alcohol use. We also did not observe an association between impulsivity and response inhibition (Reynolds et al., 2006; Dougherty et al., 2008; Caswell et al., 2013), which further suggests that it was the acute alcohol intake that impaired response inhibition in our sample.

Extending previous laboratory research on lying, we replicated the finding of an increased cognitive cost of lying in our sample (Seymour et al., 2000; Walczyk et al., 2003; Spence et al., 2008; Verschuere and De Houwer, 2011). This is important considering the need for more ecologically valid settings in forensic research (National Research Council, 2003; Evans et al., 2009). However, in contrast to our expectations, alcohol consumption was not related to the cognitive cost of lying. To interpret this finding, we have to evaluate whether our null finding may be due to a lack of power. As there is no comparable research to estimate the size of our expected effect of alcohol on the cognitive cost of lying, we used the medium-sized correlation between the BACs and SSRTs in our sample (rs = 0.35) as an estimate. Assuming the expected relationship in our sample between BACs and lying to be comparable in strength to the relationship between BACs and SSRTs, our experiment had a power of 0.93 to discover this relation. Although we cannot exclude that the size of the actual relation may be lower (e.g. as response inhibition may only be one component influencing the variance of the lie effect), we can deduct that we had reasonable power to detect a medium size effect. Another factor may be the underrepresentation of severe intoxication levels in our sample. Because of ethical reasons, every festival visitor who wanted to participate was included in the study and we did not encourage participants to drink. Although we did find an effect of alcohol on response inhibition and other research has shown that response inhibition is impaired already from moderate intoxication levels on (from 0.04%), it could be that lying is only impaired at higher alcohol levels.

It is possible that hampering effects of alcohol on lying were counteracted by other factors in our experiment. Importantly, motivational effects may have neutralized alcohol effects. It has been shown that alcohol-related impairments can be reduced when inhibition is reinforced and participants are highly motivated (Fillmore and Vogel-Sprott, 1999, 2000; Vogel-Sprott et al., 2001). Advertising our study as investigating the relation of alcohol and lying, we approached participants with the question whether they wanted to find out how well they could lie. Participants also received feedback at the end of the experiment on their ‘lying performance’ (based on their RT lie effect). Such particular motivation may have neutralized alcohol effects. Finally, it could also be the case that alcohol intake did hamper lying in our experiment, but at the same time facilitated it by decreasing moral conflict (Kireev et al., 2008; Karim et al., 2010). Sober participants may have experienced a stronger moral conflict than participants who were under the influence of alcohol and these two antagonistic effects might have counteracted each other. In that context, it may be interesting to investigate whether the use of more personal, emotionally arousing questions (e.g. Did you ever take drugs? Did you ever cheat?) would change the pattern of results. First, sober participants may experience a higher moral conflict when lying about personal, emotionally arousing questions, compared with when lying about neutral questions. Second, if alcohol intoxication reduces this moral conflict, one may observe a significant facilitation of lying for personal, emotionally arousing questions for intoxicated participants, compared with sober participants.

The present data do not support the role of response inhibition in lying. There was no association between response inhibition and lying, and alcohol did not impact on lying. As such our study may also question the role of response inhibition in lying (Gamer et al., 2012; Verschuere et al., 2012). It should be noted that so far most evidence for the contribution of response inhibition is indirect. Response inhibition has been used to explain differential effects of lying compared with truth telling, as for instance elevated RTs (Seymour et al., 2000; Verschuere and De Houwer, 2011), enlarged activation in brain areas linked to response inhibition (Spence et al., 2001; Schumacher et al., 2010; Vartanian et al., 2013) and stronger ERPs linked to conflict-detection (Johnson et al., 2004, 2005, 2008; Dong et al. 2010). More direct evidence of response inhibition during lying is scarce. Duran et al. (2010) found, that when moving a Nintendo Wii Remote to truthful or deceitful ‘yes’ or ‘no’ answers displayed on the top of a screen, participants' arm movements revealed stronger response competition for deceitful compared with truthful answers as evidenced by a stronger deviation toward the not-chosen (truthful) response. Hadar et al. (2012) found in three experiments larger motor-evoked potentials for the truthful compared with the deceitful response during preparation of a deceitful response and no such response competition during the preparation of a truthful response. But although these findings strengthen the idea that response competition indeed causes the cognitive cost of lying, they do not provide information about the specific type of inhibition needed to resolve this competition. Overcoming the truth response in lying might involve inhibition at an earlier stage than the motor inhibition required in the Stop-Signal task (also referred to as ‘action cancelation’; Sebastian et al., 2013). Hence, the inhibition involved in lying may for instance rather resemble ‘interference inhibition’ (Sebastian et al., 2013), and further deception research should differentiate and compare the subcomponents of inhibition in order to clarify which of those is involved in lying.

To sum up, this field study validates laboratory research on the acute impairing effects of alcohol on response inhibition within a realistic drinking environment. Furthermore, it replicated the increased cognitive costs of lying and provides first information on the relationship between alcohol and lying.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

Supplementary Data

Funding

This research was supported by an ECRP Grant (09-ECRP-025; FWO Grant ESF 3G099310).

Conflict of interest statement

None declared.

Acknowledgements

We thank Peter-Paul van Zenderen, Liza Ruys and Job Wagemaker for their help with the data collection. We furthermore thank the organizers of the Discovery Day and the team of the Science Center NEMO for providing us with the unique opportunity to run our study during the Discovery Day 2012.

REFERENCES

Anderson

BM

Stevens

MC

Meda

SA

et al.

Functional imaging of cognitive control during acute alcohol intoxication

Alcohol Clin Exp Res

2011

35

156

65

Aron

AR

Robbins

TW

Poldrack

RA

Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex

Trends Cogn Sci

2004

8

170

77

Aron

AR

Robbins

TW

Poldrack

RA

Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on

Trends Cogn Sci

2014

18

177

85

Bond

AJ

Drug-induced behavioural disinhibition

CNS Drugs

1998

9

41

57

Boucher

L

Palmeri

TJ

Logan

GD

et al.

Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades

Psychol Rev

2007

114

376

97

Bradley

MT

Ainsworth

D

Alcohol and the psychophysiological detection of deception

Psychophysiology

1984

21

63

71

Caswell

AJ

Morgan

MJ

Duka

T

Inhibitory control contributes to ‘motor’—but not ‘cognitive’—impulsivity

Exp Psychol

2013

60

324

34

Christ

SE

Van Essen

DC

Watson

JM

et al.

The contributions of prefrontal cortex and executive control to deception: evidence from activation likelihood estimate meta-analyses

Cereb Cortex

2009

19

1557

66

Cohen

J

Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences

1988

Hillsdale

Lawrence Erlbaum

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

de Wit

H

Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes

Addict Biol

2009

14

22

31

de Wit

H

Crean

J

Richards

JB

Effects of d-Amphetamine and ethanol on a measure of behavioral inhibition in humans

Behav Neurosci

2000

114

830

7

Debey

E

Verschuere

B

Crombez

G

Lying and executive control: an experimental investigation using ego depletion and goal neglect

Acta Psychol

2012

140

133

41

Dong

G

Hu

Y

Wu

H

The presentation order of cue and target matters in deception study

Behav Brain Funct

2010

6

63

Dougherty

DM

Marsh-Richard

DM

Hatzis

ES

et al.

A test of alcohol dose effects on multiple behavioral measures of impulsivity

Drug Alcohol Depend

2008

96

111

20

Dunlap

WP

Cortina

JM

Vaslow

JB

et al.

Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs

Psychol Methods

1996

1

170

7

Duran

ND

Dale

R

McNamara

DS

The action dynamics of overcoming the truth

Psychon Bull Rev

2010

17

486

91

Evans

JR

Compo

NS

Russano

MB

Intoxicated witnesses and suspects: procedures and prevalence according to law enforcement

Psychol Public Policy Law

2009

15

194

221

Farrow

TF

Hopwood

MC

Parks

RW

et al.

Evidence of mnemonic ability selectively affecting truthful and deceptive response dynamics

Am J Psychol

2010

123

447

53

Fillmore

MT

Acute alcohol-induced impairment of cognitive functions: past and present findings

Int J Disabil Hum Dev

2007

6

115

25

Fillmore

MT

Vogel-Sprott

M

An alcohol model of impaired inhibitory control and its treatment in humans

Exp Clin Psychopharmacol

1999

7

49

55

Fillmore

MT

Vogel-Sprott

M

Response inhibition under alcohol: effects of cognitive and motivational conflict

J Stud Alcohol

2000

61

239

46

Fillmore

MT

Ostling

EW

Martin

CA

et al.

Acute effects of alcohol on inhibitory control and information processing in high and low sensation-seekers

Drug Alcohol Depend

2009

100

91

9

Fullam

RS

McKie

S

Dolan

MC

Psychopathic traits and deception: functional magnetic resonance imaging study

Br J Psychiatry

2009

194

229

35

Furedy

JJ

Davis

C

Gurevich

M

Differentiation of deception as a psychological process: a psychophysiological approach

Psychophysiology

1988

25

683

8

Gamer

M

Verschuere

B

Ben-Shakhar

G

Meijer

E

Detection of deception and concealed information using neuroimaging techniques

Memory Detection: Theory and Application of the Concealed Information Test

2011

Cambridge

Cambridge University Press

90

114

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Gamer

M

Klimecki

O

Bauermann

T

et al.

fMRI-activation patterns in the detection of concealed information rely on memory-related effects

Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci

2012

7

506

15

Greene

JD

Sommerville

RB

Nystrom

LE

et al.

An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment

Science

2001

293

2105

08

Hadar

AA

Makris

S

Yarrow

K

The truth-telling motor cortex: response competition in M1 discloses deceptive behaviour

Biol Psychol

2012

89

495

502

Haggard-Grann

U

Hallqvist

J

Langstrom

N

et al.

The role of alcohol and drugs in triggering criminal violence: a case-crossover study

Addiction

2006

101

100

8

Hu

X

Chen

H

Fu

G

A repeated lie becomes a truth? The effect of intentional control and training on deception

Front Psychol

2012

3

488

Jentsch

JD

Taylor

JR

Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli

Psychopharmacology

1999

146

373

90

Johnson

R

Jr

Barnhardt

J

Zhu

J

The contribution of executive processes to deceptive responding

Neuropsychologia

2004

42

878

901

Johnson

R

Jr

Barnhardt

J

Zhu

J

Differential effects of practice on the executive processes used for truthful and deceptive responses: an event-related brain potential study

Brain Res Cogn Brain Res

2005

24

386

404

Johnson

R

Jr

Henkell

H

Simon

E

et al.

The self in conflict: the role of executive processes during truthful and deceptive responses about attitudes

Neuroimage

2008

39

469

82

Karim

AA

Schneider

M

Lotze

M

et al.

The truth about lying: inhibition of the anterior prefrontal cortex improves deceptive behavior

Cereb Cortex

2010

20

205

13

Kireev

MV

Pakhom*ov

SV

Medvedev

SV

Cerebral mechanisms of error detection during deceptive responses in the normal state and under the influence of alcohol

Hum Physiol

2008

34

141

9

Lappin

JS

Eriksen

CW

Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response

J Exp Psychol

1966

72

805

11

Lawrence

AJ

Luty

J

Bogdan

NA

et al.

Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling

Psychopharmacology

2009

207

163

72

Leeman

RF

Grant

JE

Potenza

MN

Behavioral and neurological foundations for the moral and legal implications of intoxication, addictive behaviors and disinhibition

Behav Sci Law

2009

27

237

59

Logan

GD

Carr

DDTH

On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a user's guide to the stop signal paradigm

Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language

1994

San Diego

Academic Press

189

239

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Logan

GD

Cowan

WB

On the ability to inhibit thought and action—a theory of an act of control

Psychol Rev

1984

91

295

327

Lyvers

M

‘Loss of control’ in alcoholism and drug addiction: a neuroscientific interpretation

Exp Clin Psychopharmacol

2000

8

225

Marczinski

CA

Fillmore

MT

Preresponse cues reduce the impairing effects of alcohol on the execution and suppression of responses

Exp Clin Psychopharmacol

2003

11

110

7

McKay

D

Schare

ML

The effects of alcohol and alcohol expectancies on subjective reports and physiological reactivity: a meta-analysis

Addict Behav

1999

24

633

47

Miyake

A

Friedman

NP

Emerson

MJ

et al.

The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis

Cognit Psychol

2000

41

49

100

Moll

J

de Oliveira-Souza

R

Eslinger

PJ

et al.

The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions

J Neurosci

2002

22

2730

6

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Moll

J

Zahn

R

de Oliveira-Souza

R

et al.

Opinion: the neural basis of human moral cognition

Nat Rev Neurosci

2005

6

799

809

Morris

SB

DeShon

RP

Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs

Psychol Methods

2002

7

105

25

Mulvihill

LE

Skilling

TA

Vogel-Sprott

M

Alcohol and the ability to inhibit behavior in men and women

J Stud Alcohol

1997

58

600

5

National Research Council

2003

Washington, DC

The National Academic Press

The polygraph and lie detection. Committee to review the scientific evidence on the Polygraph. Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education

Nigg

JT

Wong

MM

Martel

MM

et al.

Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders

J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry

2006

45

468

75

Nikolaou

K

Critchley

H

Duka

T

Alcohol affects neuronal substrates of response inhibition but not of perceptual processing of stimuli signaling a stop response

PLoS One

2013

8

e76649

O'Toole

D

Yuille

JC

Patrick

CJ

et al.

Alcohol and the physiological detection of deception: arousal and memory influences

Psychophysiology

1994

31

253

63

Oosterlaan

J

Sergeant

JA

Inhibition in ADHD, aggressive, and anxious children: a biologically based model of child psychopathology

J Abnorm Child Psychol

1996

24

19

36

Patton

JH

Stanford

MS

Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale

J Clin Psychol

1995

51

768

74

Pawliczek

CM

Derntl

B

Kellermann

T

et al.

Inhibitory control and trait aggression: neural and behavioral insights using the emotional stop signal task

Neuroimage

2013

79

264

74

Reynolds

B

Ortengren

A

Richards

JB

et al.

Dimensions of impulsive behavior: personality and behavioral measures

Pers Individ Dif

2006

40

305

15

Schachar

R

Logan

GD

Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology

Dev Psychol

1990

26

710

20

Schachar

RJ

Tannock

R

Logan

G

Inhibitory control, impulsiveness, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

Clin Psychol Rev

1993

13

721

39

Schumacher

EH

Seymour

TL

Schwarb

H

Brain activation evidence for response conflict in the exclude recognition task

Brain Res

2010

1329

113

23

Sebastian

A

Pohl

MF

Klöppel

S

et al.

Disentangling common and specific neural subprocesses of response inhibition

Neuroimage

2013

64

601

15

Seymour

TL

Schumacher

EH

Electromyographic evidence for response conflict in the exclude recognition task

Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci

2009

9

71

82

Seymour

TL

Seifert

CM

Shafto

MG

et al.

Using response time measures to assess ‘guilty knowledge’

J Appl Psychol

2000

85

30

7

Sigurdsson

JF

Gudjonsson

GH

Alcohol and drug intoxication during police interrogation and the reasons why suspects confess to the police

Addiction

1994

89

985

97

Spence

SA

Farrow

TF

Herford

AE

et al.

Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans

Neuroreport

2001

12

2849

53

Spence

SA

Kaylor-Hughes

C

Farrow

TF

et al.

Speaking of secrets and lies: the contribution of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to vocal deception

Neuroimage

2008

40

1411

8

Stevens

M

Lammertyn

J

Verbruggen

F

et al.

Tscope: A C library for programming cognitive experiments on the MS Windows platform

Behav Res Methods

2006

38

280

6

Tsujii

T

Sakatani

K

Nakashima

E

et al.

Characterization of the acute effects of alcohol on asymmetry of inferior frontal cortex activity during a Go/No-Go task using functional near-infrared spectroscopy

Psychopharmacology

2011

217

595

603

Van Bockstaele

B

Verschuere

B

Moens

T

et al.

Learning to lie: effects of practice on the cognitive cost of lying

Front Psychol

2012

3

526

52

Vartanian

O

Kwantes

PJ

Mandel

DR

et al.

Right inferior frontal gyrus activation as a neural marker of successful lying

Front Hum Neurosci

2013

7

616

Verbruggen

F

Logan

GD

Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm

Trends Cogn Sci

2008

12

418

24

Verbruggen

F

Logan

GD

Stevens

MA

STOP-IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm

Behav Res Methods

2008

40

479

83

Verschuere

B

De Houwer

J

Verschuere

B

Ben-Shakhar

G

Meijer

E

Detecting concealed information in less than a second: response latency-based measures

Memory Detection: Theory and Application of the Concealed Information Test

2011

Cambridge

Cambridge University Press

46

63

Google Scholar

OpenURL Placeholder Text

Verschuere

B

Spruyt

A

Meijer

EH

Otgaar

H

The ease of lying

Conscious Cogn

2011

20

908

11

Verschuere

B

Schuhmann

T

Sack

AT

Does the inferior frontal sulcus play a functional role in deception? A neuronavigated theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation study

Front Hum Neurosci

2012

6

284

Vince

MA

The intermittency of control movements and the psychological refractory period

Br J Psychol Gen Sect

1948

38

149

57

Vogel-Sprott

M

Easdon

C

Fillmore

M

Finn

P

Justus

A

Alcohol and behavioral control: cognitive and neural mechanisms

Alcohol Clin Exp Res

2001

25

117

21

Walczyk

JJ

Roper

KS

Seemann

E

Humphrey

AM

Cognitive mechanisms underlying lying to questions: response time as a cue to deception

Appl Cogn Psychol

2003

17

755

74

© The Author 2014. Medical Council on Alcohol and Oxford University Press. All rights reserved

Topic:

  • ethanol
  • alcohol drinking
  • deception
  • blood alcohol concentration
  • veritas trial
  • dishonesty

Issue Section:

Assessment

  • Supplementary data

  • Supplementary data

    Advertisem*nt intended for healthcare professionals

    Citations

    Views

    5,015

    Altmetric

    More metrics information

    Metrics

    Total Views 5,015

    4,168 Pageviews

    847 PDF Downloads

    Since 1/1/2017

    Month: Total Views:
    January 2017 3
    February 2017 11
    March 2017 4
    April 2017 7
    May 2017 11
    June 2017 13
    July 2017 3
    August 2017 6
    September 2017 13
    October 2017 7
    November 2017 24
    December 2017 44
    January 2018 21
    February 2018 41
    March 2018 58
    April 2018 52
    May 2018 37
    June 2018 41
    July 2018 51
    August 2018 45
    September 2018 31
    October 2018 22
    November 2018 27
    December 2018 41
    January 2019 33
    February 2019 18
    March 2019 54
    April 2019 56
    May 2019 28
    June 2019 30
    July 2019 21
    August 2019 38
    September 2019 67
    October 2019 33
    November 2019 48
    December 2019 39
    January 2020 54
    February 2020 55
    March 2020 41
    April 2020 61
    May 2020 36
    June 2020 65
    July 2020 49
    August 2020 40
    September 2020 123
    October 2020 116
    November 2020 45
    December 2020 76
    January 2021 80
    February 2021 137
    March 2021 220
    April 2021 169
    May 2021 128
    June 2021 92
    July 2021 63
    August 2021 60
    September 2021 61
    October 2021 68
    November 2021 68
    December 2021 61
    January 2022 58
    February 2022 180
    March 2022 156
    April 2022 149
    May 2022 97
    June 2022 81
    July 2022 75
    August 2022 55
    September 2022 86
    October 2022 178
    November 2022 84
    December 2022 64
    January 2023 57
    February 2023 51
    March 2023 78
    April 2023 66
    May 2023 46
    June 2023 45
    July 2023 36
    August 2023 61
    September 2023 57
    October 2023 70
    November 2023 60
    December 2023 79

    Citations

    Powered by Dimensions

    11 Web of Science

    Altmetrics

    ×

    Email alerts

    Article activity alert

    Advance article alerts

    New issue alert

    Receive exclusive offers and updates from Oxford Academic

    Citing articles via

    Advertisem*nt intended for healthcare professionals

    In Vino Veritas? Alcohol, Response Inhibition and Lying (2024)
    Top Articles
    Latest Posts
    Article information

    Author: Pres. Lawanda Wiegand

    Last Updated:

    Views: 6390

    Rating: 4 / 5 (51 voted)

    Reviews: 82% of readers found this page helpful

    Author information

    Name: Pres. Lawanda Wiegand

    Birthday: 1993-01-10

    Address: Suite 391 6963 Ullrich Shore, Bellefort, WI 01350-7893

    Phone: +6806610432415

    Job: Dynamic Manufacturing Assistant

    Hobby: amateur radio, Taekwondo, Wood carving, Parkour, Skateboarding, Running, Rafting

    Introduction: My name is Pres. Lawanda Wiegand, I am a inquisitive, helpful, glamorous, cheerful, open, clever, innocent person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.